Original Article
Contest competition and men's facial hair: beards may not provide advantages in combat

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.11.004Get rights and content

Abstract

In contemporary human societies, where direct male-male competition is reduced compared to ancestral societies, sporting competitions remain an avenue for status acquisition via intra-sexual competition. Beards are the most visually salient and sexually dimorphic of men's secondary sexual traits and play a strong role in communicating masculinity, dominance and aggressiveness intra-sexually. Hypotheses have been advanced that beards provide advantages in intra-sexual combat, as protective organs and honest signals of fighting ability. Here we provide the first test of these hypotheses using data from professional mixed martial arts fighters competing in the Ultimate Fighting Championship. We explored whether secondary sexual traits (height, weight, beardedness), fighting stance (southpaw, orthodox), arm reach and past contest experiences impact on contest outcomes. If beards function as protective organs, bearded fighters should succumb to fewer knock-outs, and hence protection to injuries to the jaw, fewer abrasions and lacerations to the face and brain damage than clean-shaven fighters. Alternatively, if beards signal fighting ability then bearded fighters should win more fights. We found no evidence that beardedness was associated with fewer losses by knock-out or greater fighter ability. While fighters with longer reaches won more fights, neither stance nor past experience influenced fight outcomes. We suggest that beards represent dishonest signals of formidability that may serve to curtail the escalation of intra-sexual conflict through intimidation rather than providing advantages in direct combat.

Introduction

Intra-sexual selection has shaped the evolution of male weaponry and aggressive behavioral displays across many taxa (Andersson, 1994, Emlen, 2008, Zahavi, 1975). Canines, and horns operate in concert with fighting styles to determine the outcome of fights (McCullough, Miller, & Emlen, 2016). Like all other animals, human males also compete intra-sexually via behavioral displays that incorporate facial and bodily secondary sexual traits (Archer, 2009, Puts, 2010, Puts, 2016). Masculine facial structure, which includes the jawline, brow ridge and mid-face, are exaggerated in men compared to women and associated with upper body strength (Fink et al., 2007, Sell et al., 2009), behavioral aggressiveness (Archer, 2009), and judgements of men's masculinity, dominance, and aggressiveness (Geniole, Denson, Dixson, Carré, & McCormick, 2015). More dominant looking adolescent males reach sexual maturity more rapidly (Doll, Cárdenas, Burriss, & Puts, 2016) and have earlier ages at first copulation (Mazur, Halpern, & Udry, 1994). In adulthood, men's acquisition of status and mating success within male-male hierarchies is associated with facial masculinity, dominance, and height (Hill et al., 2013, Muller and Mazur, 1997). This pattern is also found in small-scale subsistence societies where upper body strength is associated with male hunting reputation and reproductive success (Apicella, 2014) and social status is linked to reproductive success (von Rueden & Jaeggi, 2016). It is for these reasons that secondary sexual facial traits are argued to be shaped by contest competition in ancestral human environments (Puts, Bailey, & Reno, 2015).

Beards are arguably the most visually conspicuous and sexually dimorphic of all human secondary sexual traits. Facial hair develops under the actions of androgens during early adolescence and are fully developed by adulthood (Randall, 2008). Beards develop faster among adolescent boys who are more behaviourally aggressive (Isen, McGue, & Iacono, 2015) and intra-sexually competitive (Singal, Bhatnagar, & Kaur, 2006). In adults, bearded men report feeling more masculine (Addison, 1989) and have higher levels of serum testosterone (Knussman & Christiansen, 1988) than clean-shaven men. Full beards consistently enhance ratings of men's age, masculinity, social dominance, and aggressiveness compared to clean-shaven faces (Dixson and Brooks, 2013, Dixson and Vasey, 2012, Geniole and McCormick, 2015, Neave and Shields, 2008, Saxton et al., 2016) by augmenting jaw size (Dixson et al., 2017, Dixson et al., 2017), overall facial masculinity (Sherlock, Tegg, Sulikowski, & Dixson, 2017), and aggressive facial expressions (Dixson & Vasey, 2012). Facial hair is also more sexually attractive and is positively associated with mating success under conditions of high male-male competition (Barber, 2001, Dixson et al., 2017, Dixson et al., 2017).

Although contemporary ecological and social conditions in human societies are vastly different from those that occurred ancestrally, sporting contests remain active Darwinian arenas in which direct same-sex competition for status readily occur (Lombardo, 2012). There is also evidence that individual performance within sporting contests relies on many of the same physical and mental faculties as those employed during earlier phases of human ancestry (Lombardo, 2012). Sporting competitions are ritualised interactions typically restricted to competitions within the same-sex which are often performed in front of audiences (Deaner, Balish, & Lombardo, 2016). As would be predicted if sporting competitions were analogous to male-male competitions, analyses of 50 small-scale societies revealed that sports were more likely to occur between men than women or to be of mixed sex (Deaner & Smith, 2013). Success in sporting contests can also result in higher social status, greater mating opportunities, and higher fitness for males involved in sports (Lombardo, 2012, Postma, 2014). For example, among Sereer communities in rural Senegal, men competing in a traditional form of Greco-Roman wrestling known locally as ‘Njom’ had significantly more surviving offspring and higher social status than non-wrestlers (Llaurens, Raymond, & Faurie, 2009). This seems to be true in modern societies as well, as male athletes report higher mating success than non-athletes (Faurie, Pontier, & Raymond, 2004).

Research among mixed martial arts (MMA) fighters competing in the The Ultimate Fighting Championship® (UFC) provides evidence that sexually dimorphic traits function directly in intra-sexual competition. The UFC is a US based promotional company for MMA fights (the largest in the world) and includes the best fighters in the world. Fights take place between two individuals in an enclosed octagon-shaped cage. Fighters use a variety of techniques, generally in combination, including wrestling, kickboxing, submission grappling and boxing in no-holds barred contests in which victory is assigned via knockouts, submission, or judge decisions. Strikes to the genitals, eyes, and back of the head are forbidden, however; the lack of stringent regulations coupled with the highly aggressive nature of the fights make the UFC a potential arena for testing evolutionary hypotheses regarding male-male competition. The UFC also maintains an unofficial rule that after three losses in a row a fighter must retire (Zilioli et al., 2015), making this an intriguing arena within which to test the role of non-phenotypic traits like past contest outcomes on fighting performance. The risks involved in competing in such a violent sport are strongly financially incentivised, with winners in the UFC routinely receiving high rewards that in one fight were reported to be $400,000 (Pollet, Stulp, & Groothuis, 2013). UFC fighters with more pronounced androgen-dependent facial traits, particularly a more robust midface, have the most victories (Třebický et al., 2015, Zilioli et al., 2015) and are judged as looking more aggressive by naïve raters (Třebický et al., 2013). Participants who were unfamiliar with the UFC were also able to accurately assign winners from bouts and assigned winners higher ratings of masculinity, strength, aggressiveness and attractiveness (Little, Třebický, Havlíček, Roberts, & Kleisner, 2015). The UFC also has a female league and analyses revealed that the facial characteristics associated with male fighting success did not predict female fighting success (Palmer-Hague, Zilioli, Jagore, & DeLecce, 2016).

The UFC provides a unique opportunity to test whether beards provide any advantage to fighters or reflect a fighter's performance. While most sports that involve fights, including groups that sanction boxing and wrestling, restrict the growth of facial hair owing to views that it enhances protection against blows (Blanchard, 2009), this restriction does not apply in the UFC. Although beards cannot enhance fighting ability directly, there are two main hypotheses that suggest evolutionary advantages for facial hair during intra-sexual contests. First, phylogenetic analyses of the facial structures of ancestral members of the genus Homo suggest that facial structure and musculature evolved, in part, to protect against blows to the head (Carrier & Morgan, 2015). As facial hair develops around the jaw, lips and midface, framing and enhancing perceptions of the sexually dimorphic facial regions (Guthrie, 1970), Blanchard (2009) hypothesised that beards are analogous to the male lion's mane in covering the vital parts of the face and neck from costly strikes. Since meta-analyses of MMA bouts revealed that 66–78% of serious injuries are to the head and face (Jensen et al., 2017, Lystad et al., 2014), if the human beard has evolved as a protective organ to blows to the lower face (Blanchard, 2009), then fighters with beards should suffer fewer knockouts and technical knockouts than clean-shaven fighters.

Alternatively, as beards are incorporated into threatening jaw-thrusting behaviors (Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 2007, Guthrie, 1970), facial hair might reflect a handicap in fights that allow opponents to grasp and pull a fighter towards them, providing leverage when delivering strikes to the face (Zahavi & Zahavi, 1999). Thus, only high quality fighters can adorn full beards and may do so in order to signal confidence in their ability to overcome the handicapping effects to opponents (Zahavi & Zahavi, 1999). Beards, may also act in concert with other androgen-dependent characters like muscularity and aggressiveness to positively influence fights. Further, beards visually frame and highlight androgen-dependent masculine facial features, particularly jaw size, and therefore might influence fight outcomes via intimidating rivals. If beards are a form of handicap signal and bearded fighters have higher fighting ability compared to clean-shaven fighters, we hypothesised that bearded fighters should win more fights than clean-shaven fighters. We tested these two hypotheses using data on individual fighting characteristics (i.e. stance), morphological traits (arm reach, height and beardedness) and outcomes of contests (i.e. knockout, submission) among 395 fighters in 600 fights from UFC events 2007–2015.

Section snippets

MMA fight outcomes

Fight outcome data is available for all UFC events that have taken place, however; high-definition photographs of fighters during each event in which facial hair is easy to categorise are somewhat limited. As a result, fight outcome data was used from UFC71 (26/05/07) up to the most recent event at the time of data collection (UFC194, 12/12/2015). Fight outcomes were drawn from either the Wikipedia page for each event or the UFC official website. Method of victory was coded as follows: 1 = knock

Results

Our results are thus based on a total of 395 fighters in 600 fights where each individual had a median of 2 fights and mean of 3.04 (the distribution of fights is visualized in Supplemental Fig. 1). The frequency of each combination of fights between individuals of the different facial hair classes is summarized in Supplemental Table 1.

For our first model, we explored whether previous contest outcome, the type of facial hair, height, stance, and reach affected the outcome of a contest. The only

Discussion

Hypotheses suggest that along with playing a strong role in communicating masculinity, dominance and aggressiveness intra-sexually, beards evolved to provide advantages in intra-sexual combat through their ability to protect the face as well as being honest signals of fighting ability. Using data from 600 professional mixed martial arts (MMA) fights from 395 fighters competing in the Ultimate Fighting Championships (UFC), we show that beards neither act as a protective feature nor as an honest

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by a University of Queensland Postdoctoral Fellowship to BJWD and an ARC Future Fellowship (FT140100115) to MMK.

Data accessibility

Data are available at https://github.com/latrodektus/UFC-Beard-Data

Competing interests

We have no competing interests.

Ethical statement

This study used publically accessible data with no restrictions.

Author contributions

BJWD, JMS and MMK designed and conducted the study; WKC and MMK analyzed the data and BJWD, JMS, WKC and MMK wrote the manuscript.

References (72)

  • A. Mazur et al.

    Dominant looking male teenagers copulate earlier

    Ethology and Sociobiology

    (1994)
  • E.L. McCullough et al.

    Why sexually selected weapons are not ornaments

    Trends in Ecology & Evolution

    (2016)
  • N. Neave et al.

    The effects of facial hair manipulation on female perceptions of attractiveness, masculinity, and dominance in male faces

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2008)
  • T.V. Pollet et al.

    Born to win? Testing the fighting hypothesis in realistic fights: Left-handedness in the Ultimate Fighting Championship

    Animal Behaviour

    (2013)
  • D. Puts

    Human sexual selection

    Current Opinion in Psychology

    (2016)
  • D.A. Puts

    Beauty and the beast: Mechanisms of sexual selection in humans

    Evolution and Human Behavior

    (2010)
  • D.M. Stuart-Fox et al.

    Multiple signals in chameleon contests: Designing and analysing animal contests as a tournament

    Animal Behaviour

    (2006)
  • P.M. West et al.

    Wounding, mortality and mane morphology in African lions, Panthera leo

    Animal Behaviour

    (2006)
  • A. Zahavi

    Mate selection—A selection for a handicap

    Journal of Theoretical Biology

    (1975)
  • W.E. Addison

    Beardedness as a factor in perceived masculinity

    Perceptual and Motor Skills

    (1989)
  • M.B. Andersson

    Sexual selection

    (1994)
  • J. Archer

    Does sexual selection explain human sex differences in aggression?

    The Behavioral and Brain Sciences

    (2009)
  • J. Baker et al.

    The southpaw advantage? Lateral preference in mixed martial arts

    PLoS One

    (2013)
  • N. Barber

    Mustache fashion covaries with a good marriage market for women

    Journal of Nonverbal Behavior

    (2001)
  • D.C. Blanchard

    Of lion manes and human beards: Some unusual effects of the interaction between aggression and sociality

    Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience

    (2009)
  • D.R. Carrier

    The advantage of standing up to fight and the evolution of habitual bipedalism in hominins

    PLoS One

    (2011)
  • D.R. Carrier et al.

    Protective buttressing of the hominin face

    Biological Reviews

    (2015)
  • M. Cattelan et al.

    Dynamic Bradley–Terry modelling of sports tournaments

    Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series C

    (2013)
  • X. Chen et al.

    Pairwise ranking aggregation in a crowdsourced setting

  • R.O. Deaner et al.

    Sex differences in sports interest and motivation: An evolutionary perspective

    Evol. Behav. Scie.

    (2016)
  • R.O. Deaner et al.

    Sex differences in sports across 50 societies

    Cross-Cultural Research

    (2013)
  • A. Dixson et al.

    Sexual selection and the evolution of visually conspicuous sexually dimorphic traits in male monkeys, apes, and human beings

    Annual Review of Sex Research

    (2005)
  • B.J. Dixson et al.

    The role of facial and body hair distribution in women's judgments of men's sexual attractiveness

    Archives of Sexual Behavior

    (2016)
  • B.J. Dixson et al.

    Beards augment perceptions of men's age, social status, and aggressiveness, but not attractiveness

    Behavioral Ecology

    (2012)
  • B.J.W. Dixson et al.

    The masculinity paradox: Facial masculinity and beardedness interact to determine women's ratings of men's facial attractiveness

    Journal of Evolutionary Biology

    (2016)
  • L.M. Doll et al.

    Sexual selection and life history: Earlier recalled puberty predicts men's phenotypic masculinization

    Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology

    (2016)
  • Cited by (35)

    • Is facial structure an honest cue to real-world dominance and fighting ability in men? A pre-registered direct replication of Třebický et al. (2013)

      2022, Evolution and Human Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      Knocking an opponent unconscious requires the generation and exertion of force (i.e., gross motor output) whereas submitting an opponent might require more fine motor output (e.g., hand and wrist muscles) and winning by decision more endurance than mass (i.e., enduring a 15-min fight). Less force output is required to win by submission and decision, but submission and decision wins comprise the bulk of the total wins in MMA (Dixson, Sherlock, Cornwell, & Kasumovic, 2018). If perceived fighting ability is sensitive to cues of knockout power (i.e., muscle mass, force output), but submission and decision wins comprise the majority of total wins, then we would expect perceived fighting ability to predict the minority of total wins—knockout wins.

    • It grows on you: Perceptions of sales/service personnel with facial hair

      2021, Journal of Business Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Our focal analyses compared a salesperson with a beard versus a clean-shaven salesperson. We also included two other facial hair categories to examine their impact on our dependent measures, as beards amplify the jaw line more than other facial hair styles (Dixson et al., 2017). We predicted that having a beard (as a masculine cue) would enhance perceived expertise and trustworthiness more than not only clean-shaven but also other types of facial hair styles (e.g. a mustache without a beard).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text