Original Article
Male social status and its predictors among Garisakang forager-horticulturalists of lowland Papua New Guinea

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.05.005Get rights and content

Abstract

Social hierarchy exists in all human societies, yet the characteristics important for achieving high social status may differ cross-culturally. Studies among Western industrialized populations often use indices of socioeconomic status (SES) to determine individual social position. Conversely, studies among small-scale societies typically use locally relevant traits. Cross-cultural applicability and the relationships between these two strategies have rarely been investigated. Studies limited to industrialized societies demonstrate that low social status is associated with poor health and elevated levels of chronic stress. It remains unclear, however, if and to what extent this relationship is a recent phenomenon in industrialized Western society or a universal human characteristic that has likely been present throughout much of our recent evolutionary history. In this study, we investigate relationships between various SES and locally relevant measures of male social status, age, body-mass index (BMI), and levels of waking salivary cortisol among relatively egalitarian Garisakang forager-horticulturalists of lowland Papua New Guinea. We employ a photo-ranking method using 32 raters (16 women, 16 men) to evaluate 15 measures of social hierarchy in men (N = 32). These measures target locally relevant traits considered important for male social status (e.g., sociability, hunting skills, fighting ability, and community influence). Using principal component analysis (PCA), we extracted two components labeled as Dominance-Respect and Prosociality-Skills. Models investigating predictors of social status demonstrate that age was significantly and positively related to both Dominance-Respect and Prosociality-Skills while BMI was positively related to only Dominance-Respect. Neither Dominance-Respect nor Prosociality-Skills predicted salivary cortisol levels. However, the SES measure of Income was significantly and positively related to cortisol levels, suggesting that men with higher income experience higher levels of stress. This finding may be explained by increased psychological stress accompanying market integration among the Garisakang or, alternatively, by culturally specific expectations for resource sharing and fear of conflict. These findings emphasize the usefulness of diverse social status measures in anthropological research and stress the need for broadened perspectives regarding the causes and consequences of social status among small-scale populations.

Introduction

Social hierarchy in human and stable animal societies is a universal feature (Anderson et al., 2015, Boehm, 1999). In such contexts, social status can be defined as relative access to contested resources, which may include food, sexual or social partners or preferred locations (de Waal, 1982). High-ranking individuals are characterised by the priority of access and the ability to acquire a disproportionate share of resources. Social status in humans can influence patterns of conflict, resource allocation, mating and coordination of group tasks/decisions (Cheng et al., 2013, Cheng et al., 2010, Henrich and Gil-White, 2001). Social rank plays an important role in mating and reproduction and, thus, has important fitness consequences (Sapolsky, 2004, von Rueden et al., 2011, von Rueden and Jaeggi, 2016).

In nonhuman primates, physical strength and ability to threaten opponents play primary roles in the acquisition and maintenance of male social status (de Waal, 1982, Sapolsky, 2005). However, several additional traits appear to be important to this process, including ability to form and maintain social relationships and engage in coalitions with other group members (de Waal, 1982, Foster et al., 2009). Determinants of social status are thought to be even more numerous among humans, for whom capacity for cooperation, various social abilities and specific knowledge and expertise can play important roles in the acquisition and maintenance of male social status (Cheng et al., 2013). Thus, social status among humans is typically considered to be a multidimensional trait (Henrich and Gil-White, 2001, von Rueden et al., 2008).

Although social hierarchy is a universal feature of animal life, societies of humans and other species differ widely in degree of dominance hierarchies and the relative importance of social status in everyday lives (Boehm, 1999, Thierry, 2000). Predominantly egalitarian societies experience relatively low variation in social status and the distribution of available resources in comparison to more hierarchical societies.

Among humans, socioeconomic status (SES) has been suggested as a functional proxy for social rank (Cavigelli & Chaudhry, 2012). Human SES is traditionally based on relative income, wealth and education (Dowd, Simanek, & Aiello, 2009). Although SES and animal rank concepts are often used as analogous, they are not completely identical. For example, as for some other animals, human social status can be inherited across generations and can change over the course of the lifespan (Cavigelli & Chaudhry, 2012). On the other hand, SES as a measure of social status allows for the comparison of people that have never met, while animal ranks are assessed using face-to-face repeated social interactions among group members. Several recent studies among non-industrial, small-scale human societies have employed non-SES social status measurements based on locally and culturally relevant characteristics (Decker, 2000, von Rueden et al., 2008). Based on peer or group member ratings, these measurements are perhaps more analogous to animal ranks than SES indices.

Social status, typically measured using SES, accounts for significant health disparities among humans, particularly in developed industrialized societies (Cavigelli and Chaudhry, 2012, Dowd et al., 2009, Sapolsky, 2004). Every step down the human social ladder is thought to result in higher morbidity, although the strength of such patterns for different indicators of health vary (Cavigelli & Chaudhry, 2012). One of the physiological mechanisms that has been suggested to explain the SES-health relationship is chronic stress. Building upon previous work with nonhuman animals (Abbott et al., 2003, Creel, 2001, Sapolsky, 2005), this body of research has often reported increased levels of stress hormones and/or lower health in subordinates compared to dominant individuals (Cavigelli and Chaudhry, 2012, Sapolsky, 2004). Although acute stress reaction is potentially adaptive, chronic activation of the stress response hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis can lead to damaging effects such as cardiovascular disease, metabolic syndrome and impaired reproduction (Sapolsky, 2004). The prototypical stress hormone cortisol has often been used as a measure of the HPA activity in this work, and several studies have shown that people who occupy higher social status have significantly lower levels of cortisol (Cohen et al., 2006a, Steptoe et al., 2003, Tucker-Drob et al., 2016). This finding has been attributed to a higher rate of stressful events and fewer available options for stress release among those at the bottom of the social hierarchy (Sapolsky, 2004). However, as reviewed by Dowd et al. (2009), the relationship between SES and stress hormone levels is not consistent. Several studies have reported no or opposite relationships between SES and human cortisol levels (Brandtstdter et al., 1991, Chen and Paterson, 2006, Ranjit et al., 2005, Rosero-Bixby and Dow, 2009). Study-to-study methodological differences in cortisol laboratory analyses and SES calculations complicate the interpretation of these mixed patterns (Dowd et al., 2009). The fact that individuals living in industrialized societies are often simultaneously a part of several social hierarchies involving different members (e.g. at work, family, friends) (Sapolsky, 2005), further complicates many analyses of relationships between social status and stress. Ethnic differences in the relationship between SES and stress adds additional complexity to interpretation of the literature (Chen et al., 2015, Cohen et al., 2006b, DeSantis et al., 2007).

Studies focusing on less complex, small-scale societies (i.e., where individuals are involved in a single local social hierarchy) and assessing social status simultaneously via both SES and locally relevant, peer-based methods may help to clarify underlying relationships between social status and stress among humans. However, to date, very few studies have investigated the relationship between social status and HPA activity among small-scale societies. Decker (2000) investigated relationship among SES, peer-rated social status and cortisol levels in men living on the Caribbean island Dominica. He did not observe a relationship between cortisol and SES (income, education and material property) but did document that men who were rated as more influential, agreeable, trustworthy and helpful exhibited lower cortisol levels. Similarly, among Tsimane' forager-horticulturalists of Amazonian Bolivia, men with higher political influence have been found to have lower cortisol levels (von Rueden et al., 2014). Surprisingly, however, Tsimane' men with higher income had higher cortisol levels than their peers (von Rueden et al., 2014). These findings underscore the important implications of market integration (Godoy et al., 2005, Lu, 2007, Reyes-García et al., 2008) for studies of social status, stress and health among small-scale populations. The cultural consonance framework (Dressler et al., 2007, Dressler and Bindon, 2000) has been proposed to help explain the common negative impact of market integration on health. According to this approach, market integration challenges individual beliefs and behavior in relation to the expectations of a new cultural model, such that the inability to meet new cultural shared standards can lead to increased psychosocial stress and resultant poor health (McDade, Stallings, & Worthman, 2000).

To address limitations of previous research exploring relationships between human social status and stress, the present study seeks to investigate the multiple dimensions of male social status among the Garisakang, a small-scale society from lowland Papua New Guinea (PNG). Predominantly subsistence forager-horticulturalist, the Garisakang can be characterised as relatively egalitarian with widespread resource sharing and low social stratification. Using a study design that includes the simultaneous measurement of SES components, peer-based assessments of locally relevant individual characteristics, and measures of body-mass-index and cortisol, we investigate the nature of Garisakang social status and its relationship to stress and physical health.

Section snippets

Methods

Research approval was obtained from local village leaders, the Ethics Committee of the University of South Bohemia, and the Institutional Review Board of Harvard University. The research was conducted under a research visa issued by the National Research Institute of PNG.

Social status based on photo ranking

Photo ranking datasets from men and women raters were first analyzed separately, and the parallel analysis suggested two principal components in each cases. The following PCA with varimax rotation yielded very similar results, with the majority of items loading onto the same components. We assessed this similarity using Procrustes rotation. Congruence coefficients showed that the components were nearly identical with congruence coefficients 0.98 and 0.97 for the two components respectively

Discussion

Male social status among Garisakang forager-horticulturalists is characterised by two robust trait domains, Dominance-Respect and Prosociality-Skills. Neither of these domains was related to SES index in our sample. However, both domains were significantly impacted by individual physical condition and age. Age was positively associated with both Dominance-Respect and Prosociality-Skills, while BMI was positively related to Dominance-Respect. With regard to HPA activity, we found little support

Conclusion

The findings of the present study suggest that men's social status in a lowland PNG forager-horticulturalist society can be characterised by two main trait domains, which we have labeled Dominance-Respect and Prosociality-Skills. These two domains reflect local importance of physical strength as well as interpersonal relationship based on cooperation and sharing. Overall SES index is not a relevant measure of social status among the Garisakang. With regard to HPA activity, we found little

Data availability

The data associated with this research are available upon request from the authors. Email: [email protected]

Funding

This work was supported by the Czech Science Foundation (13-25602P) and the Faculty of Science, University of South Bohemia.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to all Garisakang, especially the Wanang village leader and our field assistants for their long-term support of our project. We thank Nigel Baro for help with questionnaire translation and the New Guinea Binatang research center and its employees for logistic support. We thank Alex Weiss (University of Edinburgh, UK) for his help with the REFA analysis, David Boukal for statistic advice and Marie Bicikova and Hana Zachystalova (Endocrinology Institute, Praha, Czech Republic) for

References (64)

  • C. von Rueden et al.

    The multiple dimensions of male social status in an Amazonian society

    Evolution and Human Behavior

    (2008)
  • R. Walker et al.

    Age-dependency in hunting ability among the ache of eastern Paraguay

    Journal of Human Evolution

    (2002)
  • C. Anderson et al.

    Is the desire for status a fundamental human motive? A review of the empirical literature

    Psychological Bulletin

    (2015)
  • R. Bird

    Cooperation and conflict: The behavioral ecology of the sexual division of labor

    Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News and Reviews

    (1999)
  • M. Bičíková et al.

    Comparison of isotope and non-isotope variant of direct cortisol immunoassay

  • C. Boehm

    Hierarchy in the forest: The evolution of egalitarian behavior

    (1999)
  • J. Brandtstdter et al.

    Developmental and personality correlates of adrenocortical activity as indexed by salivary cortisol: Observations in the age range of 35 to 65 years

    Journal of Psychosomatic Research

    (1991)
  • E. Chen et al.

    Neighborhood poverty, college attendance, and diverging profiles of substance use and allostatic load in rural African American youth

    Clinical Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science

    (2015)
  • E. Chen et al.

    Neighborhood, family, and subjective socioeconomic status: How do they relate to adolescent health?

    Health Psychology

    (2006)
  • J.T. Cheng et al.

    Two ways to the top: Evidence that dominance and prestige are distinct yet viable avenues to social rank and influence

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2013)
  • S. Cohen et al.

    Socioeconomic status is associated with stress hormones

    Psychosomatic Medicine

    (2006)
  • S. Cohen et al.

    Socioeconomic status, race, and diurnal cortisol decline in the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study

    Psychosomatic Medicine

    (2006)
  • R Core Team

    R: A language and environment for statistical computing

    (2015)
  • D. Daniels

    Magi: An undocumented language of Papua New Guinea

    Oceanic Linguistics

    (2016)
  • A. Dinno

    paran: Horn's test of principal components/factors. R package version 1.5.1

    (2012)
  • J.B. Dowd et al.

    Socio-economic status, cortisol and allostatic load: A review of the literature

    International Journal of Epidemiology

    (2009)
  • W.W. Dressler et al.

    Cultural consonance and psychological distress: Examining the associations in multiple cultural domains

    Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry

    (2007)
  • W.W. Dressler et al.

    The health consequences of cultural consonance: Cultural dimensions of lifestyle, social support, and arterial blood pressure in an African American community

    American Anthropologist

    (2000)
  • M.W. Foster et al.

    Alpha male chimpanzee grooming patterns: implications for dominance “style”

    American Journal of Primatology

    (2009)
  • P. Gibbs et al.

    Quality of life and development challenges in a Middle Ramu community

    Contemporary PNG Studies: DWU Research Journal

    (2016)
  • R. Godoy et al.

    The effect of market economies on the well-being of indigenous peoples and on their use of renewable natural resources

    Annual Review of Anthropology

    (2005)
  • B.M. Henning

    Market-based conservation in Melanesia: Contrasting expectations of landowners and conservationists

    Conservation and Society

    (2015)
  • View full text