Elsevier

Evolution and Human Behavior

Volume 38, Issue 5, September 2017, Pages 592-603
Evolution and Human Behavior

Original Article
Hunter-gatherer males are more risk-seeking than females, even in late childhood

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.01.003Get rights and content

Abstract

Observed economic and labor disparities between the sexes may, in part, result from evolved sex differences in risk preferences. Using incentivized economic games, we report on sex differences in risk preferences in the Hadza, a population of hunter-gatherers. One game played in 2010 (n = 233) found that more Hadza males than females prefer to gamble for a chance to earn more maize rather than settle for a sure, but smaller, amount. Similarly, a second game played in 2013 (n = 102) found that male Hadza gamble a greater proportion of honey for a chance to earn more compared to female Hadza. Effect sizes are small to medium. We find weak evidence that risk-taking increases in men as their mating opportunities increase. In both games, the sex difference widens throughout childhood and is greatest among adolescents; though note that child samples are small. We explore developmental trends further using observational data on food returns in children (n = 357). Our data suggest that while the mean number of calories boys bring to camp remains stable with age, the variance in their caloric returns increases. Among girls, the variance remains stable with increased age. Both the economic games and food return data are consistent with the sexual division of labor wherein boys, beginning in late childhood, begin to target riskier foods. To the extent that the Hadza allow us to make inferences about long-standing patterns of human behavior, we suggest that sex differences in risk preferences may have been present long before agriculture and the modern work environment.

Introduction

Financial and labor disparities are observed between the sexes in all types of societies. Men typically attain more powerful, higher earning positions (Blau and Kahn, 2000, DiPrete and Buchmann, 2013, Bertrand and Hallock, 2001), despite the fact that in some societies and sectors women are, on average, more educated (Altbach et al., 2009, Vincent-Lancrin, 2008). In 2014 for example, women working full-time earned about 79% of what men did (Blau & Kahn, 2016). While decompositions of the gender wage gap illustrate the importance of a variety of factors, a sizable disparity between men and women's pay remains unexplained by traditional economic variables (Blau & Kahn, 2016). As such, some researchers have suggested that labor inequalities may, in part, result from innate sex differences in psychological preferences such as willingness to compete, initiate negotiations, or take on risk (e.g., Apicella and Dreber, 2015, Babcock et al., 2006, Croson and Gneezy, 2009, Eckel and Grossman, 2008, Powell and Ansic, 1997, Saad, 2011).

While sex differences for some preferences, such as competitiveness, are generally large and robust, the results for risk-taking have been more heterogeneous, though most studies report a sex difference (for detailed review, Niederle, in press). Byrnes, Miller, and Schafer (1999) conducted a meta-analysis of 150 studies spanning three decades that compared men and women in various types of risk-taking behavior (e.g., driving and gambling). While the majority of risk categories showed a sex difference, many of the effects were small. Using a hypothetical questionnaire, where evolutionarily typical risks were presented using modern frames, Wang, Kruger, and Wilke (2009) found that men reported more risk-taking in all domains studied. Eckel and Grossman (2008) and Croson and Gneezy (2009) provide two broad reviews of studies that ask individuals to either make decisions between alternatives that vary in monetary risk or to make valuations of risky payoffs. Both reviews conclude that men generally exhibit higher levels of risk-taking but acknowledge that less consistent evidence is found when gambles are framed as losses. However, two more recent surveys of sex differences in risk-taking that analyzed studies with different methods of eliciting risk preferences reached markedly different conclusions about the existence of sex differences, suggesting that the method of elicitation may account for heterogeneity of the findings (Charness and Gneezy, 2012, Filippin and Crosetto, 2016).1 The latest and most comprehensive survey of the literature (e.g., Niederle, in press) concludes that while sex differences in risk do likely exist, the difference may not be substantial in all settings.

A few cross-cultural examinations have been conducted. Charness and Gneezy (2012) assembled data from a number of studies, including students living in Sweden, USA and Turkey and villagers from China, India and Tanzania, and conclude that in 90% of the experiments men make relatively larger investments in risky assets. Cárdenas, Dreber, Von Essen, and Ranehill (2012) find that boys, ages 9–12, in both Sweden and Colombia were more risk-taking than their female counterparts. Similarly, Buser, Niederle, and Oosterbeek (2014) report that Dutch male high school students chose a significantly more risky lottery than did girls. Finally, a survey conducted with nearly 7000 university students, largely studying economics, from 53 different countries find significant and robust differences between men and women in their propensity to take risks in the gain domain (Rieger, Wang, & Hens, 2014). The opposite finding emerged when looking at losses. In this domain, women were less risk-averse. Again, this finding is not particularly surprising given the inconsistent results previously reported when gambles are framed as losses (e.g., Croson & Gneezy, 2009).

Although there is growing and admirable interest in how individuals living in small-scale societies make decisions involving risk, the majority of studies have relied on Western populations and students, leaving the generalizability and origins of these sex differences unknown. Here, we report on sex differences in risk preferences among the Hadza, one of the only remaining populations of hunter-gatherers who rely primarily on wild foods for subsistence. Given that the Hadza live in a social and physical environment that more closely approximates that of human origins than industrialized environments, their risk preferences may provide insight into the origins of sex differences (Apicella and Dreber, 2015, Apicella et al., 2007, Cashdan et al., 2012). If nothing else, the Hadza offer a valuable example of how men and women living in a subsistence economy – without farming or livestock – make decisions under conditions of risk.

Anthropologists, economists, and psychologists have used the term risk to mean different things. In the current study, we formalize risk preferences in the tradition of economics to represent the tradeoff between the variance and the expected value for a given resource. Our measures, therefore, involve asking participants to make choices between options that are less rewarding but more certain, and options that are less certain, but potentially more rewarding. This study focuses on risk in the domain of gains.

Nearly all species have evolved in environments with a substantial element of unpredictability. Consequently, decisions involving uncertain outcomes in diverse behaviors such as mating, foraging, or parenting are ubiquitously observed across taxa, and the outcomes of such behaviors can have measurable fitness consequences. In hunter-gatherers, for instance, decisions are made daily about which foods to target for consumption. Foods such as meat are risky since variance in hunting returns is high, while other foods, such as foraged plant items, are more reliably procured but may be less energy dense (Cordain et al., 2001, Smith, 1988). Outside of the traditional realms of ethological study one can find analogies in the decision making of humans in industrialized societies, from career decisions to medical treatment options. In all of these instances, the degree to which choices have consequences to fitness, evolution can be expected to have an underlying role.

To the extent that men and women confronted different challenges in the past and that the same choices made under conditions of uncertainty would have yielded different returns based on sex, it is possible that natural selection shaped sex differences in risk preferences. The standard narrative for many observed sex differences from ornamentation (e.g., Darwin, 1871) to mate choice (e.g., Buss, 1989) is that they are evolutionary downstream consequences of sex differences in the patterning of reproduction, in which the costs of reproducing are higher for females (Trivers, 1972). This difference leads to higher potential rates of reproduction and reproductive skew for men (Bateman, 1948, Trivers, 1972); the corollary of this is that men are designed to compete for mates since this is the limiting factor for their reproductive success. Apicella et al. (2008), suggest that financial risk-taking may be a modern form of male-male competition for resources, which can then be used to attract mates. More generally, Wang et al. (2009) argue that increased acceptance of risk-taking in males facilitates male-male competition. Formalized evolutionary models for sex differences in risk preferences have been provided. Dekel and Scotchmer (1999) argue that sex differences in risk-taking will be selected in winner-take-all environments, where top males mate with the majority of females. Rubin and Paul (1979) show that in environments where only those males who are above a certain income threshold are attractive to females, sex difference in risk preferences will evolve. Robson (1996) expands on this model by including repeated thresholds that correspond to increasingly larger number of mates and shows that if males choose lotteries over wealth, they will choose very risky lotteries, if any. In hunter-gatherers, hunting for big game may be akin to choosing risky lotteries.

For nearly all forager populations for which detailed ethnographic data exist, we see a marked sexual division of labor wherein men primarily target high-risk resources such as game animals and women primarily target plant resources, the staple of the diet (Kelly, 2013).2 The fact that better hunters experience greater reproductive success in a number of these societies, including the Hadza (Apicella, 2014, Hawkes, 2001, Marlowe, 1999), the Ache (Hill and Hurtado, 1996, Kaplan and Hill, 1985) and !Kung (Wiessner, 2002) supports the notion that hunting ability, which requires a protracted period of learning (Gurven et al., 2006, Blurton Jones and Marlowe, 2002), was evolutionarily selected. While women do value hunting ability in their mates (Marlowe, 2003a, Marlowe, 2003b, Marlowe, 2004a, Apicella and Crittenden, 2016), there is lively debate over how hunting increases men's reproductive success. It has long been viewed that hunting evolved to provision a man's pair-bonded family unit (e.g., Washburn & Lancaster, 1968). Indeed, evidence suggests that a hunter's family benefits directly by receiving choice cuts of meat – even in settings of communal sharing (Wood & Marlowe, 2013) – and by obtaining nutrients and protein not found in foraged foods (Domínguez-Rodrigo et al., 2013), but alternative explanations have been proposed. Specifically, it had been argued that hunting may instead serve as a costly signal for advertising mate quality (Hawkes and Bliege Bird, 2002, Smith, 2004) and/or building a reputation for generosity useful for fostering cooperative friendships (Gurven, Allen-Arave, Hill, & Hurtado, 2000). Since game meat is less reliable and shared widely in hunter-gatherer groups, these are plausible explanations. How this debate is settled may have implications for how we understand the evolution of sex differences in risk preferences in humans if we think that the sexual division of labor reinforced or further promoted sex differences in risk-taking. That is, foraging may itself provide an additional selection pressure, but sex differences in risk-taking were likely present before the sexual division, as evinced by observations in numerous other species.

While different interpretations of Hadza men's foraging goals persist, we believe that insight can be generated from examining: 1) whether sex differences are present in the Hadza and, if so, 2) when during development they emerge. A developmental approach, notably, does not promise to isolate the role of culture versus biology on the development of sex differences. However, if sex differences emerge early in development, this provides some indication that the difference may be innate. Conversely, when sex differences emerge later in development, culture may contribute (e.g., social learning) but biological forces (e.g., puberty) cannot be ruled out. Still, combining developmental and cross-cultural data can be valuable when trying to identify species-typical features of our psychology (Apicella & Barrett, 2016).

Here we use two economic games to measure risk preferences and supplement these games by also analyzing variance in foraging yield as a more natural, albeit indirect, measure of risk-taking in children and adolescents. As boys get older, usually around the age of ten, they spend less time foraging with women and girls and begin to pursue game meat, which is more risky. For these reasons, we expect to see increased variance in food returns with age in boys but not girls. While these data would accord with a shift from stable to risky foraging behavior, the weakness of these data is that we neither know the particular foods targeted and subsequently missed, nor how much children worked/ate when away from camp.

There are some reasons to suspect that the characteristic sex difference in risk preferences may not be observed in the Hadza. Compared to many societies in the world, Hadza women enjoy a high level of autonomy, are vocal, and participate in making important decisions at the camp level (Marlowe, 2010). And a number of studies suggest the social and economic organization of groups is critical for the emergence of sex differences for some economic preferences. For instance, Booth and Nolen (2012) find that girls who attend same-sex schools are more likely to choose real stakes lotteries over sure bets compared to girls attending mixed-sex schools. Another study that highlights the role of culture finds that a gender difference in competitiveness observed among adults in a patriarchal society in Northeast India arises at puberty, but such a sex difference is not found in a nearby village that practices matrilineal inheritance (Andersen, Ertac, Gneezy, List, & Maximiano, 2013). In contrast however, Gong and Yang (2012) find that men are more risk-seeking in both matrilineal and patriarchal groups in China.

Studies have also examined biological determinants of risk preferences. For instance, it has been suggested that prenatal androgen exposure may influence risk preferences in adulthood, and consequently may explain some of the observed sex difference (for critique see Apicella, Carré and Dreber, 2015). These studies largely correlate digit ratio measurements (i.e., second to fourth digit or 2D:4D), a purported proxy of androgen exposure in utero, with risk preferences. While in a majority of populations 2D:4D is sexually dimorphic with males having a lower ratio than females, the Hadza do not exhibit this pattern (Apicella, Tobolsky, Marlowe, & Miller, 2016). In fact, the evidence suggests that, if anything, women have more masculine digit ratios (but see Butovskaya, Burkova, & Mabulla, 2010). It is unknown whether hormones underlie this unusual pattern of 2D:4D in the Hadza, but to the extent they do and to the extent that the association between 2D:4D and risk-taking is real, we may find that Hadza men and women behave similarly when faced with uncertainty. Likewise, current testosterone levels may also influence economic risk preferences – but this link too, is uncertain (Apicella et al., 2015) – and available evidence suggests that Hadza men may have lower testosterone than men in neighboring groups, such as the Datoga (Muller, Marlowe, Bugumba, & Ellison, 2009). This finding accords with observations that high investment in a pair-bond (Burnham et al., 2003, Gray et al., 2004, McIntyre et al., 2006) and higher degrees of parental investment (Gray, Kahlenberg, Barrett, Lipson, & Ellison, 2002) tend to correlate with lower testosterone levels in men. While hormonal measurements have not been taken in Hadza women, Cashdan (2008) has argued that increased androgens might be favored in Hadza women, as evidenced by more masculine phenotypes, since their work is physically demanding, requiring strength and stamina. Accordingly, we would not be surprised to find Hadza men and women exhibiting comparable decision-making strategies when confronted with risk.

Section snippets

Subjects

The Hadza are a population of nomadic hunter-gatherers living in a savannah-woodland environment in Northern Tanzania. The Hadza live in temporary camps of about 30 individuals, but both camp residence and camp locations shift in response to resource availability. For this reason, many individuals know each other even when not currently living in the same camp (Apicella, Marlowe, Fowler, & Christakis, 2012). The Hadza target five main categories of wild foods: meat, honeycomb, baobab, berries,

Results

Table 1 provides demographic statistics for the two risk games. Roughly 66% of male Hadza chose to take the gamble in the first game involving maize. This contrasts with the women and girls, wherein only 43.7% chose to take the gamble (see Fig. 2, Panel A). A chi-square test of independence was performed to examine the relation between sex and risk-taking in the maize game for the full sample of participants. The results indicate that a significantly greater proportion of male Hadza chose to

Discussion

Evidence from the current study shows that male Hadza are more likely to opt for a risky strategy rather than settle for a reduced but certain outcome as compared to female Hadza. About 33% more male Hadza chose to risk their maize in the first study. Likewise, males risked about 17% more of their endowed honey for a chance to earn more. Assuming the Hadza embody similar behavioral patterns to those used by many human societies before the advent of agriculture (see Apicella & Crittenden, 2016

Contributions

C.L.A., A.N.C. and V.A.T. wrote the paper. C.L.A. conceived of the study. C.L.A. and A.N.C. collected data. A.N.C. calculated caloric values. C.L.A. performed the statistical analyses.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Deus Haraja, Ibrahim Mabulla, Mika Peterson, Golden Ngumbuke, and Happy Msofe for their research assistance, Audax Mabulla for his continued support and friendship and Ian Smith and Brian Wood for feedback and advice. C.L.A. and A.N.C. would also like to thank Frank W. Marlowe for his advice and mentorship over the years. The research was made possible through funds provided by the University of Pennsylvania, the John Templeton Foundation (Science of Generosity

References (126)

  • J.T. Cheng et al.

    Pride, personality, and the evolutionary foundations of human social status

    Evolution and Human Behavior

    (2010)
  • A.N. Crittenden et al.

    Juvenile foraging among the Hadza: Implications for human life history

    Evolution and Human Behavior

    (2013)
  • E. Dekel et al.

    On the evolution of attitudes towards risk in winner-take-all games

    Journal of Economic Theory

    (1999)
  • A. Dreber et al.

    Beauty queens and battling knights: Risk-taking and attractiveness in chess

    Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization

    (2013)
  • C.C. Eckel et al.

    Sex differences and statistical stereotyping in attitudes toward financial risk

    Evolution and Human Behavior

    (2002)
  • C.C. Eckel et al.

    Men, women and risk aversion: Experimental evidence

    Handbook of Experimental Economics Results

    (2008)
  • B. Gong et al.

    Gender differences in risk attitudes: Field experiments on the matrilineal Mosuo and the patriarchal Yi

    Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization

    (2012)
  • P.B. Gray et al.

    Marriage and fatherhood are associated with lower testosterone in males

    Evolution and Human Behavior

    (2002)
  • M. Gurven et al.

    “It's a wonderful life”: signaling generosity among the Ache of Paraguay

    Evolution and Human Behavior

    (2000)
  • M. Gurven et al.

    How long does it take to become a proficient hunter? Implications for the evolution of delayed growth

    Journal of Human Evolution

    (2006)
  • F.W. Marlowe

    A critical period for provisioning by Hadza men: Implications for pair bonding

    Evolution and Human Behavior

    (2003)
  • F.W. Marlowe et al.

    Honey, Hadza, hunter-gatherers, and human evolution

    Journal of Human Evolution

    (2014)
  • S.S. Murray et al.

    Nutritional composition of some wild plant foods and honey used by Hadza foragers of Tanzania

    Journal of Food Composition and Analysis

    (2001)
  • J.M. Plavcan et al.

    Interpreting hominid behavior on the basis of sexual dimorphism

    Journal of Human Evolution

    (1997)
  • M. Powell et al.

    Gender differences in risk behaviour in financial decision-making: An experimental analysis

    Journal of Economic Psychology

    (1997)
  • A.J. Robson

    The evolution of attitudes to risk: Lottery tickets and relative wealth

    Games and Economic Behavior

    (1996)
  • P.G. Altbach et al.

    Trends in global higher education: Tracking an academic revolution

    (2009)
  • S. Andersen et al.

    Gender, competitiveness, and socialization at a young age: Evidence from a matrilineal and a patriarchal society

    The Review of Economics and Statistics

    (2013)
  • C.L. Apicella

    High levels of rule bending in a minimally religious population of hunter-gatherers

    Religion, Brain and Behavior

    (2017)
  • C.L. Apicella et al.

    Hunter-Gatherer families and parenting

  • C.L. Apicella et al.

    Sex differences in competitiveness: Hunter-gatherer women and girls compete less in gender-neutral and male-centric tasks

    Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology

    (2015)
  • C.L. Apicella et al.

    Voice pitch predicts reproductive success in male hunter-gatherers

    Biology Letters

    (2007)
  • C.L. Apicella et al.

    Social networks and cooperation in hunter-gatherers

    Nature

    (2012)
  • C.L. Apicella et al.

    Evolutionary origins of the endowment effect: Evidence from hunter-gatherers

    American Economic Review

    (2014)
  • C.L. Apicella et al.

    Testosterone and economic risk-taking: A review

    Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology

    (2015)
  • C.L. Apicella et al.

    Hadza hunter-gatherer men do not have more masculine digit ratios (2D:4D)

    American Journal of Physical Anthropology

    (2016)
  • D. Ariely et al.

    The heat of the moment: The effect of sexual arousal on sexual decision making

    Journal of Behavioral Decision Making

    (2006)
  • L. Babcock et al.

    Gender differences in the propensity to initiate negotiations

  • A.J. Bateman

    Intra-sexual selection in Drosophila

    Heredity

    (1948)
  • J.C. Berbesque et al.

    Sex differences in food preferences of Hadza hunter-gatherers

    Evolutionary Psychology

    (2009)
  • M. Bertrand et al.

    The gender gap in top corporate jobs

    Industrial and Labor Relations Review

    (2001)
  • F.D. Blau et al.

    Gender differences in pay

    The Journal of Economic Perspectives

    (2000)
  • F.D. Blau et al.

    The gender wage gap: extent, trends and explanations

  • N. Blurton Jones
    (2016)
  • N.G. Blurton Jones et al.

    Selection for delayed maturity: Does it take 20 years to learn to hunt and gather?

    Human Nature

    (2002)
  • S. Bogandov et al.

    Honey for nutrition and health: A review

    American College of Nutrition

    (2008)
  • A.L. Booth et al.

    Gender differences in risk behaviour: does nurture matter?

    The Economic Journal

    (2012)
  • J. Brown

    A note on the division of labor by sex

    American Anthropologist

    (1970)
  • T. Buser et al.

    Gender, competitiveness and career choices

    The Quarterly Journal of Economics

    (2014)
  • D.M. Buss

    Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures

    Behavioral and Brain Sciences

    (1989)
  • Cited by (37)

    • A cost for signaling: do Hadza hunter-gatherers forgo calories to show-off in an experimental context?

      2023, Evolution and Human Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      A study of resource prioritization in three forager populations (the Ache, Martu and Meriam; Codding et al., 2011) showed that men were willing to take on greatly more risk of failure in pursuit of high-energy resource types than were women, especially when high-energy resources were unreliably attained. Moreover, results are further consistent with research among the Hadza showing strong sex differences both in the proclivity for risk-taking in games of chance (Apicella et al., 2017) and in the motivation to compete (Apicella & Dreber, 2015). Although men and women both found signaling to be important and gave precedence to family provisioning as a self-reported motive for foraging, it is notable that men are more likely to rank signaling as a primary motive than women (Stibbard-Hawkes et al., 2022).

    • Sex differences in trust and trustworthiness: A meta-analysis of the trust game and the gift-exchange game

      2020, Journal of Economic Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      In short, it pays for men to be relatively more risk-taking in contexts where they can acquire resources like status, goods and money. This is evidenced by many studies that find men to take more (social) risks than women across different ages and cultures, and from modern to traditional societies (Apicella, Crittenden, & Tobolsky, 2017; Byrnes, Miller, & Schafer, 1999; Fischer & Hills, 2012; Wilson & Daly, 1985). Differences in risk-taking might help explain sex differences in trust because trust (as defined above) involves a willingness to be vulnerable to other people’s adverse behaviors (i.e., to take social risks).

    • Cross-cultural, developmental psychology: integrating approaches and key insights

      2020, Evolution and Human Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      Several recent examples of work blending ethnographic and standardized experimental approaches highlight the utility of combining these approaches. First, Coren Apicella, Crittenden, and Tobolsky (2017) studied risk-seeking behavior in Hadza children using a combination of standardized economic games and observational data on food returns. They then linked sex differences in children's preferences in the economic games to sex differences in their foraging behavior, finding that Hadza males are more risk-seeking than females, even in late childhood.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text