Original Article
No relationship between intelligence and facial attractiveness in a large, genetically informative sample

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.11.009Get rights and content

Abstract

Theories in both evolutionary and social psychology suggest that a positive correlation should exist between facial attractiveness and general intelligence, and several empirical observations appear to corroborate this expectation. Using highly reliable measures of facial attractiveness and IQ in a large sample of identical and fraternal twins and their siblings, we found no evidence for a phenotypic correlation between these traits. Likewise, neither the genetic nor the environmental latent factor correlations were statistically significant. We supplemented our analyses of new data with a simple meta-analysis that found evidence of publication bias among past studies of the relationship between facial attractiveness and intelligence. In view of these results, we suggest that previously published reports may have overestimated the strength of the relationship and that the theoretical bases for the predicted attractiveness–intelligence correlation may need to be reconsidered.

Introduction

Some evolutionary models predict that traits contributing to survival or reproductive success will tend to be positively correlated (Hansen, 2006, Rowe and Houle, 1996; but, see Falconer and Mackay, 1996, Lerner, 1954). Examples of such positive correlations have been observed in humans [IQ and sperm quality: Arden, Gottfredson, Miller, and Pierce (2009); IQ and height: Keller et al. (2013); birth rate, completed family size, and age at last childbirth: Kosova, Abney, and Ober (2010)], animals [energy storage and metabolic activity in Drosophila melanogaster: Clark (1990); body weight across environmental niches in Alsophila pometaria: Futuyma and Philippi (1987); activity metabolism and locomotor performance in Thamnophilis sirtalis: Garland (1988)], and plants [size and pest resistance in Ipomoea purpurea: Rausher and Simms (1989); life history and morphological traits in Holcus lanatus: Billington, Mortimer, and McNeilly (1988); life history and morphological traits in Impatiens capensis: Mitchell-Olds (1986)]. There are two basic types of explanation for why these correlations occur. One is that the conditions in the environment, such as pathogen levels or the availability of adequate nutrition, have similar effects on both of the correlated traits (Møller, 1997). The other is that the phenotypic correlation is caused by a correlation between the effects of the alleles influencing the two traits (Falconer & Mackay, 1996).

Genetic correlations, in turn, can come about in two principal ways. One is pleiotropy, whereby a gene affects multiple phenotypic characters. Pleiotropy is a common property of genes (Falconer & Mackay, 1996) and is a proposed explanation for genetic correlations between a large number of medical (Flint and Mackay, 2009, Solovieff et al., 2013) and psychological (Kovas and Plomin, 2006, Lee et al., 2013) traits, many of which appear to be highly polygenic (e.g., Davies et al., 2011, Purcell et al., 2009, Stahl et al., 2012). Antagonistic pleiotropy, whereby alleles that improve one fitness-related trait deteriorate another fitness-related trait, can lead to stable genetic polymorphism and persistent negative genetic correlations between fitness-increasing traits. However, the conditions under which genetic polymorphism is maintained by antagonistic pleiotropy are restrictive (Hedrick, 1999, Prout, 2000), and most investigations in non-human animals have found positive rather than negative correlations between fitness-increasing traits (Roff, 1997). On the other hand, to the degree that the genetic variation in directionally selected traits is due to the aggregate effects of deleterious mutations across many loci (Houle, 1998), genetic correlations between fitness-increasing traits should be positive. Under this scenario, pleiotropic loci that affect two or more fitness-increasing traits should tend to harbor common alleles that are favored by selection and rare mutations that are selected against because they negatively affect both traits.

Even when the traits are affected by non-overlapping sets of genes, a second possible cause of genetic correlations is assortative mating on two or more traits simultaneously, which can lead to non-random associations between alleles at different loci (i.e., gametic phase disequilibrium; Crow & Felsenstein, 1968). To the degree that overall attractiveness is a composite of multiple sexually selected traits, positive assortment between mates on overall attractiveness necessarily implies positive cross-trait correlations between traits positively related to attractiveness. When this occurs, individuals who inherit alleles that increase the sexual attractiveness of one trait from one parent will be more likely to inherit alleles that increase the sexual attractiveness of the other trait from the other parent, leading to positive genetic correlations between sexually selected traits when scaled such that scores increase with attractiveness.

Both intelligence (Miller, 2000) and facial attractiveness (Gangestad et al., 1994, Thornhill and Gangestad, 1999) have been hypothesized to be sexually selected traits related to fitness, perhaps because their large mutational target sizes (Davies et al., 2011) reveal a partner's load of deleterious mutations (Gangestad and Yeo, 1997, Keller, 2007, Miller, 2000). If so, then as described above, there are two basic explanations for why facial attractiveness and intelligence might be expected to be positively genetically correlated. First, because these traits are influenced by a large number of genes, there is likely to be some degree of overlap between them. While such overlap could lead to negative genetic correlations from antagonistic pleiotropy, the restrictive conditions under which antagonistic pleiotropy can maintain negative genetic correlations at equilibrium (Hedrick, 1999, Prout, 2000) suggest that a better expectation is that pleiotropic loci lead to positive genetic correlations via transiently polymorphic, recurrent deleterious mutations that reduce both intelligence and facial attractiveness. Second, given that people rate both facial attractiveness and intelligence as desirable in romantic and sexual partners (Buss and Barnes, 1986, Buss et al., 1990, Kenrick et al., 1990), it is also possible that cross-trait assortative mating (intelligent people choosing more facially attractive mates, and vice-versa) produces statistical associations between alleles affecting the two traits. These two possible causes of genetic correlations are not mutually exclusive; for example, Keller et al. (2013) used an extended twin-family design that accounted for the genetic effects of assortative mating to demonstrate that both processes contributed roughly equally to the genetic correlation between human height and IQ.

Several social psychological theories also predict a correlation between intelligence and facial attractiveness. For instance, status generalization theory holds that visible characteristics affecting social status, including facial attractiveness, cause perceivers to generate matching expectations about other traits of the target (Jackson, Hunter, & Hodge, 1995)—for example, more attractive individuals are assumed to be more intellectually and socially competent, to have more integrity and compassion, and so on (Eagly et al., 1991, Moore et al., 2011). Although this theory primarily predicts correlations between visible status cues and perceived levels of internal characteristics, Jackson et al. (1995) argue that, due to the more positive evaluations attractive individuals receive in social and intellectual contexts, they may also receive more opportunities to develop intellectual competence than unattractive individuals. Moreover, attractive individuals may form self-concepts based on social feedback that include notions of superior intellectual ability, potentially motivating intellectual achievement (L. A. Jackson et al., 1995). Thus, both social psychological and evolutionary considerations seem to predict, a priori, a positive phenotypic correlation between intelligence and facial attractiveness.

A survey of the published studies on intelligence and attractiveness is summarized in Table 1.The general pattern, identifiable in Jackson et al.'s (1995) and Langlois et al.'s (2000) meta-analyses, appears to be that a small-to-moderate correlation is found in children (r¯ = .19, weighted by sample size), but the relationship diminishes with age (r¯ = .02, weighted by sample size). However, interpretation of these meta-analytic results is difficult, not only because meta-analyses are vulnerable to the “file drawer problem”, whereby null results are less likely to be published than positive ones (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2011), but also because of inconsistencies in operational definitions of intelligence and attractiveness across included studies and because many of the included studies had design flaws (e.g., non-independence of intelligence and attractiveness ratings) that could have created biases in the results (Table 1).

Several empirical studies have tested the attractiveness–intelligence correlation since these meta-analyses were published. In contrast to the pattern noted above, Zebrowitz and Rhodes's (2004) found a moderate positive correlations in both children and adults, but only among individuals with below-median attractiveness levels; averaging together the correlations in high- and low-attractiveness groups likely would have resulted in effects more consistent with the earlier meta-analyses. Similarly, Denny (2008) showed that low intelligence may predict low attractiveness in a large sample of school children but that “[f]or much of the distribution of intelligence there is no significant relationship between being attractive and intelligence” (p. 618). Kanazawa (2011) analyzed two large samples of children and young adults, including the one from Denny (2008). Controlling for parental education and income, birth weight, age at puberty, and physical health reduced but did not eliminate the association he observed between physical attractiveness and general intelligence. A serious limitation of the Denny (2008) and Kanazawa (2011) studies is that the raters of attractiveness were familiar with targets' intelligence, leading to potential rater biases that may have artificially induced the correlation under investigation [e.g., see Moore et al. (2011) for a demonstration of an intelligence ‘halo’ effect on perceived attractiveness.] Most recently, Kleisner, Chvátalová, and Flegr (2014), using reliable, independently collected measures of intelligence and facial attractiveness, failed to find a statistically significant correlation in either male or female young adults. However, this study, like those of Zebrowitz and Rhodes (2004) and many studies included in Jackson et al.'s (1995) and Langlois et al.'s (2000) meta-analyses, utilized a very small sample, rendering its results somewhat inconclusive.

Finally, we observe that nine of 41 previously reported correlations (22 %) were negative and that only 17 of 41 (41 %) were statistically significant. If there truly is some level of positive correlation between intelligence and facial or physical attractiveness, both of these outcomes could reflect high sampling variance due to the small samples most past studies have employed (median N = 83; Table 1). Although the literature in general seems to affirm that a correlation exists (median r = .09; Table 1), the validity of any such meta-analytic result depends both on the individual included studies utilizing appropriate research methods and on the meta-analysis as a whole being free of biases, including publication bias.

The present study is the first to utilize highly reliable and independently collected measures of facial attractiveness and general intelligence in a sample much larger than most individual studies in the past have had access to. Importantly, our study also utilizes a genetically informative twin dataset, allowing us to partition the covariation between attractiveness and intelligence into its genetic and environmental components.

Section snippets

Overview

We combined data from two twin samples to test the hypothesis that facial attractiveness and intelligence are correlated. The first sample comprised participants (n = 399) enrolled in the Longitudinal Twin Study (LTS), previously described by Rhea et al., 2006, Rhea et al., 2012. The second sample included participants (n = 1,354) enrolled in the Brisbane Adolescent Twin Study (BATS), previously described by Wright and Martin (2004). Attractiveness data for both samples were collected at the same

Phenotypic analyses

As an overall estimate of the relationship between facial attractiveness and IQ, we obtained a standardized regression coefficient (equivalent to a partial correlation) of β = .018 (p = .50). This effect was similar in both sexes: when the facial attractiveness × sex interaction was included, neither facial attractiveness (β = .016, p = .55) nor the interaction (β =  .040, p = .33) was a significant predictor of IQ. In both models, the effect of facial attractiveness did not differ significantly between the

Discussion

Existing evolutionary genetic models predict that persistent directional selection and cross-trait assortative mating can produce positive genetic covariance between traits (Hansen, 2006), and it has been shown that IQ and height are positively correlated due to both pleiotropy and cross-trait assortative mating (Keller et al., 2013). We had expected that facial attractiveness and intelligence would be subject to even stronger cross-trait assortative mating, and in view of previous empirical

Supplementary materials

The following is the supplementary data to this article.

11-2014 SCRIPT fa_iq_MS.

lts_data_anon.

atiq_meta-analysis.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by grants from the Australian Research Council (A79600334, A79801419, and DP0212016), the National Institutes of Health (HD010333), and the National Institute of Mental Health (MH085812 and MH63207). The authors would like to thank the Australian Twin Registry and Colorado Twin Registry twins and their families for their continued participation. We would also like to thank Marlene Grace, Ann Eldridge, Daniel Park, and David Smyth for their work with the Australian Twin

References (65)

  • S.M. Boker et al.

    OpenMx: An open source extended structural equation modeling framework

    Psychometrika

    (2011)
  • Michael Borenstein et al.

    Introduction to meta-analysis

    (2011)
  • D.M. Buss et al.

    International preferences in selecting mates: A study in 37 cultures

    Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology

    (1990)
  • D.M. Buss et al.

    Preferences in human mate selection

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1986)
  • Andrew G. Clark

    Genetic components of variation in energy storage in Drosophila melanogaster

    Evolution

    (1990)
  • J.F. Crow et al.

    The effect of assortative mating on the genetic composition of a population

    Biodemography and Social Biology

    (1968)
  • G. Davies et al.

    Genome-wide association studies establish that human intelligence is highly heritable and polygenic

    Molecular Psychiatry

    (2011)
  • A.H. Eagly et al.

    What is beautiful is good, but…: A meta-analytic review of research on the physical attractiveness stereotype

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1991)
  • D.S. Falconer et al.

    Introduction to quantitative genetics

    (1996)
  • Richard B. Felson

    Physical attractiveness, grades and teachers' attributions of ability

    Representative Research in Social Psychology

    (1980)
  • J. Flint et al.

    Genetic architecture of quantitative traits in mice, flies, and humans

    Genome Research

    (2009)
  • I.H. Frieze et al.

    Attractiveness and income for men and women in management

    Journal of Applied Social Psychology

    (1991)
  • D.J. Futuyma et al.

    Genetic variation and covariation in responses to host plants by Alsophila pometaria (Lepidoptera: geometridae)

    Evolution

    (1987)
  • M.T. Gabriel et al.

    Narcissistic illusions in self-evaluations of intelligence and attractiveness

    Journal of Personality

    (1994)
  • T. Garland

    Genetic basis of activity metabolism. I. Inheritance of speed, stamina, and antipredator displays in the garter snake Thamnophis sirtalis

    Evolution

    (1988)
  • Thomas F. Hansen

    The evolution of genetic architecture

    Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics

    (2006)
  • P.W. Hedrick

    Antagonistic pleiotropy and genetic polymorphism: A perspective

    Heredity

    (1999)
  • P.W. Hedrick

    Genetics of populations

    (2011)
  • D. Houle

    How should we explain variation in the genetic variance of traits?

    Genetica

    (1998)
  • D.N. Jackson

    MAB, multidimensional aptitude battery: Manual

    (1984)
  • L.A. Jackson et al.

    Physical attractiveness and intellectual competence: A meta-analytic review

    Social Psychology Quarterly

    (1995)
  • M.C. Keller

    The role of mutations in human mating

  • Cited by (18)

    • Assortative mating and the evolution of desirability covariation

      2019, Evolution and Human Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      A second important future direction is resolving the mixed findings in this broader research area. For instance, although in our samples physical attractiveness, intelligence, and health load onto the d-factor in theoretically consistent ways, some studies have failed to find correlations between these traits (e.g. Feingold, 1992; Mitchem et al., 2015). These inconsistencies must be explained.

    • Assessing the accuracy of perceptions of intelligence based on heritable facial features

      2017, Intelligence
      Citation Excerpt :

      These photographs were rated on a number of traits, such as facial attractiveness, facial masculinity, and trustworthiness. For the analyses presented here, we focus on ratings of perceived intelligence (for more detail on the rating process, see Mitchem et al., 2015). For perceived intelligence, photographs were presented in a random order to one of two groups of undergraduate research assistants (21 in total; 12 Females, 9 Males; 19–30 years, median = 22 years).

    • Physical attraction to reliable, low variability nervous systems: Reaction time variability predicts attractiveness

      2017, Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      Given the necessarily imperfect correlation between reaction time variability and IQ, the effect of g on facial appearance would have to be very large to account for the whole of our effects. However, recent studies do not show strong support for the claim that facial appearance provides valid cues to IQ (Mitchem et al., 2015; Talamas, Mavor & Perrett, 2016). At present, therefore, psychometric g is an unlikely explanation of our effects.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text