Elsevier

Evolution and Human Behavior

Volume 33, Issue 5, September 2012, Pages 551-556
Evolution and Human Behavior

Original Article
Male facial width is associated with death by contact violence: narrow-faced males are more likely to die from contact violence

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.02.002Get rights and content

Abstract

Male facial width-to-height ratio (bizygomatic width scaled for face height) is a testosterone-linked trait predictive of reactive aggression, exploitative behavior, cheating, deception, and dominance. We tested whether facial width was systematically related to cause of death in a forensic sample. We hypothesized that wider-faced males, being more aggressive and robust, would be less likely than narrower-faced males to die from contact violence (stabbed, strangled, or bludgeoned to death) compared with other forms of homicide. We tested this hypothesis in a forensic data sample covering 523 male and 339 female skeletons. In these data, men with narrower faces were more likely to have died as a consequence of homicides involving direct physical contact than men with wider faces. No such effect was found for women. This effect was found when considering all causes of mortality and when limiting the sample to homicides. This finding suggests that wider-faced males are less likely to die from male–male physical violence, perhaps because of their formidability. Our findings are discussed with reference to the previous literature indicating that facial width-to-height ratio is a marker for male dominance.

Introduction

Ratings of male facial dominance from photographs accurately predict fighting ability (Sell et al., 2009). Facial dominance can also be predictive of life history. For example, the facial dominance of West Point US Military cadets in the 1950s predicted their final military rank as well as the number of children that they would sire (Mueller & Mazur, 1996). Typical features of facial dominance, associated with perceived lack of warmth, lack of honesty, and lack of cooperation, have been masculinity (Perrett et al., 1998) and prominent cheekbones (Cunningham, Barbee, & Pike, 1990).

Recently, relationships have been found between male dominance, reactive aggression, and variation in cheekbone measurements. Variation in human male bone growth, specifically cranial growth, is related to testosterone effects in adolescence (Verdonck, Gaethofs, Carels, & de Zegher, 1999), and studies have shown that male bone growth across the zygomatic arches (bizygomatic width), measured as the ratio of facial width to height, is related to perceptions of aggression and exploitativeness (Carre et al., 2009, Carre et al., 2010, Stirrat and Perrett, 2010). This measure also predicts actual aggression (Carre & McCormick, 2008, but see Özener, in press), cheating and deception (Haselhuhn & Wong, 2011), and exploitation of others (Stirrat & Perrett, 2010) in competitive and economic contexts, as well as increased self-reported ability to win “a physical fight with a same sex peer”.

Fighting ability, or strength, also predicts a man's willingness to express anger towards others (Sell, Tooby, & Cosmides, 2009) and is therefore also likely to predict violence. Indeed, male facial width has been positively associated with measures of actual physical violence: in one study, !Kung San Bushmen with broader bizygomatic measures had been more often involved in violent interactions resulting in head scars than those men with narrower bizygomatic width (Christiansen & Winkler, 1992). The authors interpreted the head scars as a measure of willingness to engage in violent fights because, amongst other measures, testosterone measures were positively associated with the number of scars in these violent men.

Signaling dominance and deciphering signals of dominance appear to have evolved in several nonhuman species [e.g., Harris sparrows (Rohwer, 1975)] to reduce the costs of competing over dominance. Accurately assessing a rival and avoiding a fight are likely to be less costly than losing a fight with a severe injury (Dawkins & Krebs, 1978). Signals of dominance may evolve in conditions where the cost of fighting (e.g., death) is greater than the benefit of the resource that is fought over (Maynard Smith & Harper, 1988)—although recent models suggest that signaling may be stable even over very high value resources and low costs of conflict where there are also high levels of commitment to defending that resource (see Szamado, 2011). As it has been found in a number of small-scale human societies that up to a third of men die as a result of violent encounters (Keeley, 1996), we might therefore expect that a cue to physical dominance such as facial width-to-height ratio would be a relevant parameter in human evolution. Given the foregoing comments about associations between facial width and anger (Sell, Tooby et al., 2009), violence (Christiansen & Winkler, 1992), and dominance (Mueller & Mazur, 1996), it would seem likely that wider-faced men are more willing to enter aggressive encounters and that they will do so because they are more likely to survive such encounters.

We therefore set out to test whether wider-faced men are more likely to survive aggressive encounters. We predicted that men who die from physically violent encounters, such as beating, strangulation, blunt force trauma, or stabbing, are likely to have a narrower facial width-to-height ratio when compared with those who die from technological causes such as poisoning or gunshot wounds, which would not favor men with either wide or narrow faces. We tested this hypothesis in a forensic anthropology data set (Jantz & Moore-Jansen, 2006). This database has detailed measurements of skeletal remains associated with cause of and age at death. We predicted that the skeletal remains of male homicide victims, who died from contact violence, would have narrower facial width-to-height ratios than those who died from other causes. More importantly, within homicide victims, we predicted that those who died from contact violence would have a lower relative facial width-to-height ratio than those who died from other causes of homicide (such as gunshot wounds). Even if wider-faced males are more likely to engage in aggressive encounters, we predicted them to be better able to survive direct physical confrontations but not better able to survive more indirect forms of violence. We also predicted that there would be no such relationship between female facial width-to-height ratio and cause of death.

Section snippets

Data

We used data from a database of forensic anthropology in the United States (Jantz & Moore-Jansen, 2006). This set contains detailed forensic data on 1514 skeletons. This collection aimed to represent different ethnic and socioeconomic groups from different regions across the United States. The data contain skeletons from diverse sources including (forensic) anthropology departments, the National Museum of Natural History, the Medical Examiner's Office, and various other sources. A full

Results

The descriptive statistics for the sample can be found in Table 1.

Although facial width-to-height ratio has previously been found to differ between the sexes (Weston, Friday, & Lio, 2007), males in this sample did not have a larger mean facial width-to-height ratio than females [M=.0032; S.E.=.008; t(860)=.379; p=.705].

Discussion

In line with our predictions, we found that narrow-faced men in this sample were more likely than wider-faced men to die by contact violence compared with other causes of death or homicide. This was the case even though we might expect wider-faced men to get into more fights. Also in line with our predictions, there was no evidence of an association between dying from contact violence and facial width-to-height ratio in women. Our results suggest that facial width-to-height ratio is a valid

Supplementary Materials

The following is the Supplementary data to this article.

AdditionAlanalysisOnlineMaterial.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to Jantz and Moore-Jansen for making these data publicly available via the ICPSR (doi:10.3886/ICPSR02581). The views presented in this paper are those of the authors and not those of the ICPSR or the original collectors of the data. Thomas Pollet is supported by The Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (Veni scheme; 265 451.10.032).

References (23)

  • J.M. Carre et al.

    In your face: facial metrics predict aggressive behaviour in the laboratory and in varsity and professional hockey players

    Proceedings of the Royal Society. Series B, Biological Sciences

    (2008)
  • J.M. Carre et al.

    Facial structure is a reliable cue of aggressive behavior

    Psychological Science

    (2009)
  • J.M. Carre et al.

    Estimating aggression from emotionally neutral faces: which facial cues are diagnostic?

    Perception

    (2010)
  • K. Christiansen et al.

    Hormonal, anthropometrical, and behavioral-correlates of physical aggression in Kung-San men of Namibia

    Aggressive Behavior

    (1992)
  • M.R. Cunningham et al.

    What do women want—facialmetric assessment of multiple motives in the perception of male facial physical attractiveness

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1990)
  • M. Daly et al.

    Homicide

    (1988)
  • R. Dawkins et al.

    Animal signals: information or manipulation?

  • M.P. Haselhuhn et al.

    Bad to the bone: facial structure predicts unethical behaviour

    Proceedings of the Royal Society. Series B

    (2011)
  • R.J. Jantz et al.

    Database for forensic anthropology in the United States, 1962–1991: Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research (ICPSR) [distributor]

    (2006)
  • L.H. Keeley

    War before civilization

    (1996)
  • E.T. Lee et al.

    Statistical methods for survival data analysis

    (2003)
  • Cited by (65)

    • Is facial width-to-height ratio reliably associated with social inferences?

      2021, Evolution and Human Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, there are several reasons to question the theoretical core of fWHR research and the ostensible associations between fWHR, behavioral tendencies, and social perceptions. First, while Carré and McCormick (2008) found evidence of sexual dimorphism in fWHR from facial photographs mirroring Weston et al. (2007), subsequent studies suggest that fWHR is not reliably sexually dimorphic in humans (Kramer, 2017; Kramer et al., 2012; Lefevre et al., 2012; Özener, 2012; Robertson & Kingsley, 2018; Stirrat, Stulp, & Pollet, 2012; but see Köllner, Janson, & Schultheiss, 2018). Second, although early research found some support for hypothesized links between testosterone and fWHR (Lefevre, Lewis, Perrett, & Penke, 2013), large-scale studies and meta-analyses using a broad range of methodologies fail to support associations between testosterone and fWHR (Bird et al., 2016; Eisenbruch, Lukaszewski, Simmons, Arai, & Roney, 2018; Hodges-Simeon, Sobraske, Samore, Gurven, & Gaulin, 2016; Kordsmeyer, Freund, Pita, Jünger, & Penke, 2019).

    • Physical formidability and acceptance of police violence

      2021, Evolution and Human Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      Those who expect to triumph in a fight are more apt to accept violent norms than are those who expect defeat, and being physically powerful tends to create such expectations (Sell et al., 2017; Sell, Eisner, & Ribeaud, 2016). Similarly, past experience of success in combat derives from formidability and associates with increased likelihood of supporting the use of violence to resolve situations (Stirrat, Stulp, & Pollet, 2012), even among government decisionmakers as they choose policies (Horowitz, Stam, & Ellis, 2015). This association between formidability and embrace of aggressive behavior fits naturally within the logic of asymmetrical competition in evolutionary biology (Maynard Smith & Parker, 1976), including in humans (Puts, 2010).

    • The evolution of pride and social hierarchy

      2020, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology
    • Does the primate face cue personality?

      2023, Personality Neuroscience
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text