Original Articles
The multiple dimensions of male social status in an Amazonian society

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.05.001Get rights and content

Abstract

While social-status hierarchies are common to all human societies, status acquisition is relatively understudied in small-scale societies lacking significant material wealth or intergenerational inheritance. Among the Tsimane of Bolivia, a small-scale Amazonian society, we employ a photo-ranking methodology to determine the important predictors of four measures of male social status: success in dyadic physical confrontation, getting one's way in a group, community-wide influence, and respect. The predictors evaluated include age, physical size, skill in food production, level of acculturation, prosocial personality, and social support. We find that physical size best predicts rankings of dyadic fighting ability while social support best predicts getting one's way in a group, community-wide influence, and respect. Level of acculturation, furthermore, is an independent predictor of influence but not respect, and skill in food production is an independent predictor of respect but not influence. The lack of a linear relationship between age and the polyadic social-status measures is evaluated in light of the increasing exposure of the Tsimane to market economies and public education among recent age cohorts. To our knowledge, this study is the first multivariate analysis of social status that considers different determinants of status simultaneously.

Introduction

“Behaviour that's admired is the path to power among people everywhere.”

-Beowulf, a new verse translation by Seamus Heaney (2000, p. 5)

In all human societies, individuals differ in social status depending upon their age and personal ability (Sahlins, 1958, Service, 1971). In laboratory-based small group studies, status hierarchies emerge spontaneously (Bass, 1954, Campbell et al., 2002, Kalma, 1991). Even among “egalitarian” foragers, who are characterized by widespread resource sharing (Kaplan & Gurven, 2005, Winterhalder, 1986) and some degree of status leveling (Cashdan, 1980), certain individuals consume more resources, get the best pick of mates, and take a more central role in group decision making (Boehm, 1999, Trigger, 1985, Wiessner, 1996). Whether implicit or overt, classification by social status is a human universal. While women as well as men compete for status (Campbell, 2002, Hess & Hagen, 2006, Hrdy, 1999, Rucas et al., 2006), this article focuses exclusively on male-status hierarchies.

Social status can be defined as relative access to resources within a social group (Henrich & Gil-White, 2001). A priority of resource access is granted to high-status individuals, we argue, due to a group-wide perception that these individuals have a greater relative ability to inflict costs (i.e., dominance) or confer benefits (i.e., prestige) on others. Group members acquiesce to higher-status individuals because they believe they will avoid harm or gain some benefit from their deference. Status hierarchies, therefore, are not necessarily pure zero-sum arrangements. In part, status hierarchies represent agreements, maintained by deference signals, to facilitate exchange or to avoid costs of repeated contest competition, as modeled by the war of attrition (Maynard Smith & Price, 1973).

Status hierarchies are not static, however. If an individual becomes less dependent upon the services of particular high-status group members, that individual is perhaps more likely to challenge power inequities between them (Emerson, 1962). Acquiescence to those of high status may also vary with subordinates' ability to migrate and their degree of relatedness with high-status individuals (Vehrencamp, 1983). Subordinates can also form coalitions against higher-status individuals. In many traditional human societies, exploitive leaders are often ridiculed, ostracized, or killed (Boehm, 1999).

Among nonhuman primates, male social status is in large part tantamount to the ability to inflict costs; physically dominant individuals usurp or maintain priority access to food and mates (Boesch et al., 2006, Cowlishaw & Dunbar, 1991, de Waal, 2000). While alliances can be important among nonhuman primates, they are largely to support the above uses of dominance (de Waal, 2000, Duffy et al., 2007, Nishida & Hosaka, 1996). Among humans, however, the cooperative sharing of food, information, labor, and other resources is extensive (Kaplan & Gurven, 2005). Priority access to resources is only sometimes obtained through dominance and is often mediated by voluntary transfers of inalienable commodities. Henrich and Gil-White (2001) contrast dominance with prestige, which they describe as the deference that accrues to individuals who possess or transmit publicly esteemed skills. Their discussion of prestige, however, is narrower than the potential range of non-agonistic social status. For example, a man may achieve high status in the market for mates by offering “good genes” or material goods in exchange for sexual access, or he may gain status in the market for influence by offering physical strength or coordinative leadership to potential allies.

Since humans have lived in hunter-gatherer societies for the majority of their existence, modern forager communities can help elucidate the selective forces responsible for human males' status-seeking behavior. Forager societies typically lack major wealth accumulation or formal political or legal institutions (Kelly, 1995). As a result, physical dominance may play a principal role in acquiring male status across social domains. Among foragers who practice some degree of horticulture and who engage in intergroup raiding, warriorship is a primary avenue to community-wide influence (Yanomami: Chagnon, 1988; Achuar: Patton, 2000; Waorani: Robarchek & Robarchek, 1998). On the other hand, status acquisition in forager societies has also been linked to attributes like hunting ability (Tsimane: Gurven & von Rueden, 2006; Ache: Kaplan & Hill, 1985; Hadza: Marlowe, 2000; Mbuti: Turnbull, 1965) and generosity (Achuar: Patton, 2005; Yuqui: Stearman, 1989). It is unclear whether male-status hierarchies in forager societies are truly multidimensional: do traits predictive of dyadic dispute outcomes, for example, contrast or even trade off with those traits predictive of polyadic influence? Understanding the pursuit of status in different social contexts is crucial precisely because the adaptiveness of this pursuit is often unclear and the rewards of pursuit may be delayed and vary with the form of status acquired.

This study explores male-status hierarchies among the Tsimane forager-horticulturalists of Bolivia. Since status is most palpable in the context of contest competition, we treat the following measures as manifestations of social status among Tsimane adult males: (a) success in dyadic physical confrontation, (b) getting one's way in the context of a conflict within a group, and (c) influence in the context of a community-wide dispute. We also investigate (d) respect because this term is cross-culturally associated with social status. These four measures of social status were chosen because they represent distinct assemblages of social costs and benefits within small-scale societies, assay relative resource access at different social scales, and lend themselves to empirical evaluation and cross-cultural comparison. None of these measures should be considered equivalent to dominance or prestige; in all likelihood, each of the status measures will reflect elements of both. Furthermore, our four measures are not meant to be exhaustive of the range of social status among the Tsimane.

While prior ethnographies have demonstrated a link between one particular trait and social status (e.g., Stearman, 1989), this study employs a multivariate approach to compare several traits as predictors of status in different social domains. If the predictors of one of our four status measures differ from those of the other status measures, we could conclude that status hierarchies among Tsimane are multidimensional. The predictors of male status we evaluate include age, physical size (e.g., flexed bicep circumference), skill in food production (e.g., hunting ability), level of acculturation (e.g., Spanish fluency), prosocial personality traits (e.g., generosity in meat sharing), and social support (e.g., number of allies). To quantify these variables as well as the social-status measures, a sample of Tsimane men photo-ranked their fellow villagers. We hypothesized that body size would best predict winning a dyadic physical fight while the other predictors of status, especially ratings of social support, would better predict the other social-status measures that involve n-person interactions.

The organization of this article is as follows: Section 2 discusses the socioecology of the Tsimane to further motivate our choice of the four status measures; Section 3 presents our hypotheses concerning the predictors of the four social-status measures; Section 4 describes our methods; Section 5 presents our results; Section 6 interprets the results and describes the status characteristics of two recent Tsimane leaders; and Section 7 concludes.

Section snippets

The socioecology of the Tsimane of Bolivia

The Tsimane inhabit areas of lowland Bolivia along the Maniqui River and in adjacent forests. While families may spend weeks or months on hunting or field cultivation trips away from settled villages, the Tsimane are semisedentary and live in communities ranging from 30 to 500 individuals. Most food the Tsimane consume derives from horticulture, fishing, hunting, and gathering activities. They cultivate plantains, rice, corn, and sweet manioc in small swiddens and regularly fish and hunt for

Hypotheses

The type of disputes particular to the dyad, group, and community will affect which variables best predict social status in those contexts. Among the Tsimane, dyadic fights often involve alcohol and are usually motivated by accusations of theft, stinginess, or sexual jealousy. Size and strength are primary determinants of successful fight outcomes (Archer, 1988), and flexed bicep circumference among U.S. college students was recently found to predict lifting strength and self-reported success

Photo-ranking

All analyses are of the entire adult male population from one of the more acculturated Tsimane villages, Ton'tumsi. There were 57 adult men over 18 years old among a total village population of approximately 300 individuals. At the time of data collection, only 8 of the 57 men were unmarried and no one had more than one wife.

To generate rankings of our status measures and their predictors, we asked Tsimane males to photo-rank other males in their community. Status hierarchies are less

Success in dyadic physical confrontation

The regression model is significant and explains 71.1% of the variance in winning dyadic fights. As anticipated, the strongest predictor of winning fights is the physical-size factor. Height is probably least responsible for this relationship since it loads the least on the physical-size factor among the four physical-size variables (see Appendix 2 in the Supplementary Material). Additionally, height produces a smaller bivariate correlation with success in dyadic fights (r=.571, n=53, p<.001)

Interpretation of the results

Among Tsimane men of Ton'tumsi, physical size is the primary determinant of dyadic fight outcomes. Social support is slightly more predictive than physical size of getting one's way in a group. Greater social support and acculturation result in community-wide influence, and greater social support and skill in food production (i.e., hunting ability) generate respect. Larger physical size, greater acculturation, and prosocial behavior are independently associated with more social support, and

Conclusion

Social status may be viewed as a growth process in which one invests to gain fitness-related benefits over the life course. Across small-scale societies, gains in status enable men to marry a younger, more fecund wife (e.g., Hadza: Marlowe, 2000; Meriam: Smith et al., 2003; Ifalukese: Turke & Betzig, 1985); marry more wives (e.g., Kipsigis: Borgerhoff Mulder, 1987; Yanomamo: Chagnon, 1988; Aka: Hewlett, 1988); or engage in more extramarital affairs (e.g., Ache: Kaplan & Hill, 1985), because

Acknowledgments

C.V.R. thanks Lisa McAllister and Helena von Rueden for methodological advice, John Tooby and Aaron Sell for inspiration, and the residents of Ton'tumsi, Bolivia, for their patience and participation. Steve Gaulin, Ruth Mace, Erik Marsh, Robert Walker, and three anonymous reviewers provided helpful comments on an earlier draft of this article.

References (99)

  • Reyes-GarciaV.

    The origins of monetary income inequality: Patience, human capital, and division of labor

    Evolution and Human Behavior

    (2007)
  • RucasS.

    Female intrasexual competition and reputational effects on attractiveness among the Tsimane of Bolivia

    Evolution and Human Behavior

    (2006)
  • TurkeP.W. et al.

    Those who can do: Wealth, status, and reproductive success on Ifaluk

    Ethology and Sociobiology

    (1985)
  • VehrencampS.

    A model for the evolution of despotic versus egalitarian societies

    Animal Behaviour

    (1983)
  • WalkerR.

    Age dependency in hunting ability among the Ache of eastern Paraguay

    Journal of Human Evolution

    (2002)
  • WiessnerP.

    Hunting, healing, and hxaro exchange: A long-term perspective on !Kung (Ju/'hoansi) large-game hunting

    Evolution and Human Behavior

    (2002)
  • WinterhalderB.

    Diet choice, risk, and food sharing in a stochastic environment

    Journal of Anthropological Archaeology

    (1986)
  • AdamsJ.

    The Gitksan potlatch: Population flux, resource ownership, and reciprocity

    (1973)
  • ArcherJ.

    The behavioral biology of aggression

    (1988)
  • BaileyR. et al.

    Significance of the social relationships of Efe pygmy men in the Ituri Forest, Zaire

    American Journal of Physical Anthropology

    (1989)
  • BaltesP.B. et al.

    Towards a psychology of wisdom and its ontogenesis

  • BarnettH.G.

    The nature of the potlatch

    American Anthropologist

    (1938)
  • BassB.M.

    The leaderless group discussion

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1954)
  • BassB.M.

    Stodgill's handbook of leadership

    (1981)
  • BlauP.M.

    Exchange and power in social life

    (1964)
  • BoehmC.

    Hierarchy in the forest: The evolution of egalitarian behavior

    (1999)
  • BoeschG.

    Male competition and paternity in wild chimpanzees of the Tai Forest

    American Journal of Physical Anthropology

    (2006)
  • BooneJ.

    Competition, conflict, and development of social hierarchies

  • BooneJ.

    The evolution of magnanimity: When is it better to give than to receive?

    Human Nature

    (1998)
  • Borgerhoff MulderM.

    On cultural and reproductive success: Kipsigis evidence

    American Anthropologist

    (1987)
  • BowlesS.

    Equality's fate: Toward a natural history

    (2005)
  • CampbellA.

    A mind of her own: The evolutionary psychology of women

    (2002)
  • CampbellL.

    The formation of status hierarchies in leaderless groups: The role of male waist-to-hip ratio

    Human Nature

    (2002)
  • Case, A. & Paxson, C. (2006). Stature and height: Height, ability, and labor market outcomes. National Bureau of...
  • CashdanE.

    Egalitarianism among hunters and gatherers

    American Anthropologist

    (1980)
  • Central Intelligence Agency. (2008). The world factbook,...
  • ChagnonN.

    Life histories, blood revenge, and warfare in a tribal population

    Science

    (1988)
  • CostelloA. et al.

    Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: Four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis

    Practical Assessment, Research, and Evaluation

    (2005)
  • Daillant, I. (1994). Sens dessus-dissous: Organisation sociale et spatial des Chimanes d'Amazonie boliviane. Ph.D....
  • DeNavas-Walt, C., et al. (2006). Income, poverty, and health insurance in the United States: 2005. U.S. Census Bureau...
  • de WaalF.

    Chimpanzee politics: Power and sex among apes

    (2000)
  • DuffyK.G.

    Male chimpanzees exchange political support for mating opportunities

    Current Biology

    (2007)
  • EllisL.

    The high and the mighty among man and beast: How universal is the relationship between height (or body size) and social status?

  • ElmendorfW.

    Coast Salish status ranking and intergroup ties

    Southwestern Journal of Anthropology

    (1971)
  • EmersonR.M.

    Power–dependence relations

    American Sociological Review

    (1962)
  • FafchampsM.

    Solidarity networks in pre-industrial societies: Rational peasants with a moral economy

    Economic Development and Cultural Change

    (1992)
  • FrankR.H.

    Passions within reason

    (1988)
  • GodoyR.A.

    Do markets worsen economic inequalities? Kuznets in the bush

    Human Ecology

    (2004)
  • GodoyR.A.

    Signaling by consumption in a native Amazonian society

    Evolution and Human Behavior

    (2006)
  • Cited by (226)

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    Funding for Tsimane research was provided by grants from the National Science Foundation (#BCS-0136274 and BCS-0422690) and National Institutes of Health/National Institute on Aging (#1R01AG024119-01).

    View full text